Directions

A School/Community Library "Book Flood"
Experiment in Western Samoa1
GA.Wagner and Dick Bishop2
Introduction
During the last decade or so, research in Fiji into the acquisition of English as a
Second Language (ESL) has determined that: (1), a quarter of Class 6 school
children were unable to read simple English with functional understanding (Elley
and Mangubhai, 1979; Elley and Achal, 1980); and (2), large numbers of primary
school children were unable to read the textbooks and other school materials
designed specifically to aid them in their school studies (Stamp, 1979; Elley,
1980).
What these studies drew attention to was a serious deficiency in the way that
English as a second language was being taught in Fiji schools. The widespread
use of the Oral English Syllabus model of language instruction (Tate, 1971) has
been identified as a major contributing factor to the poor performance of Fiji
pupils in English language tests (Elley and Mangubhai, 1983). In the latter's
view: "The oral drills practised by the pupils have little justification as a form of
genuine communication..." (p.55). Serious though this is for Fiji, what should be
realised is that the Tate model for ESL teaching is still being widely used
throughout the central Pacific.
There is, however, increasing evidence which points to better ways for children to
learn communication skills, especially in a second language. It appears, from
1 This paper is based upon information contained in a report by Graham Wagner, Dick Bishop and Mataina Te'o
for the IOE/USP entitled The Fagamalo School/Community Library Research Project, Studies in South Pacific
Education No. 5, NZCER/IOE-USP, December 1987 (Revised July 1991).
2 The authors wish to acknowledge the helpful editorial comments made by Ian Livingstone (NZCER),
Warwick Elley (University of Canterbury), Cliff Benson (IOE/USP) and Barbara Moore (IOE/USP).
60

research on language development that providing children with regular
opportunities to read for pleasure (Smith, 1978; Lado, 1977), along with hearing
their parents and teachers reading aloud (Moore, 1986), exposes these learners to "a
rich range of stories" which has the consequent and significant effect of improving
their language skills (Chomsky, 1979; Lado, 1977).
We know too from research on the Shared Book Method of learning English in
Niue (Elley, 1980; De'Ath, 1980) and a study of a comparison between the Shared
Book method and Silent Reading method in Fiji (Elley and Mangubhai, 1981) that
these methods of improving first language learners' language skills also apply to
ESL primary school children in some Pacific cultures. The necessary requirement
for improvement to take place is a generous source of interesting books and a
process by which children can interact with these books in a natural and conducive
fashion (Elley, 1991).
Unfortunately, in most Fiji primary schools (Ragni, 1979), and in the experience
of the authors who have both travelled and worked extensively in South Pacific
island nations, there are very few English language story books in the USP
region's schools. Nevertheless, following on from the Elley-initiated research into
teaching ESL in the early 1980's there has been a strong interest in exploring the
possibility that if any school in the region was flooded with interesting books3,
there was a good chance that its children would significantly improve their English
skills.
Given that English story books are often expensive, hard-to-come-by, and usually
appeal to adults as well as children, it made good sense to undertake a study that
looked at sharing a flood of books through a school/community library placed in a
Pacific Island school where there would be the likelihood of cooperation between
a local school and the community in the use of these resources. With this plan in
mind, the then Director of the Institute of Education initiated a research proposal
Approximately 250 were used in the Elley and Mangubhai (1981) study.
61

through the USP to UNESCO for funding4. The application was successful, a
contract was entered into and the research commenced in 1984.5
Aim, Objective and Hypotheses
Aim
Under the terms of a contract between UNESCO and the University of the South
Pacific (USP), the Institute of Education, in conjunction with the Western Samoa
Public Library Service6, was commissioned to investigate whether the introduction
of a library into a school, with few such story books, would make a significant
difference over a period of time to the reading comprehension skills of its pupils.
The Research Objective/Hypotheses
Given the nature of the contract, there were three principal objectives: (1) a
language study, (2) a school/community library feasibility survey and (3) an
investigation of the procedures for establishing a joint-use library in a rural
community. This paper reports on the first objective regarding "the relevance of
library and information resources in developing and improving school children's
reading levels".
To this end, and for this part of the study, it was hypothesised that the
introduction and use of a school/community library in a small rural community
would improve the ESL skills of the pupils at a local junior high school
(experimental school) compared with a junior high school without such a library
4 Francis Mangubhai, the Director of the IOE/USP at the time told the first author that the idea for Book Flooding
a school through a library came up during a regional conference for librarians run by the South Pacific Commission
in Fiji in 1983. At the same time it was decided to seek a site for the Book Hood outside Fiji. Western Samoa was
selected for the reasons mentioned later in this paper.
' This paper covers only one section from a larger study on the impact of a Book Flood through the agency of a
school/community library in Western Samoa (Wagner, Bishop and Te'o, 1991). To better see the implications of this
paper, with reference to the success of the school/community library model used in Western Samoa, recourse should
be made to the entire study available through the Institute of Education, USP, Suva, Fiji.
6 See contract details mentioned in the Wagner, Bishop and Te'o (1991) report.
62

(control school). A subsidiary hypothesis was that third form pupils in the top
English class at the experimental school would improve their ESL skills to a
higher level of competence if exposed to regular "formal" Silent Reading sessions
than those pupils in the other English class at the same school, or any English
class at the control school, where no such systematic and regular silent reading
took place.
Method
Design
A "before" and "after" experimental design was employed, with pre-and post-tests
administered to experimental and control groups (ie., schools and classes). Using
a control group made it possible to more reliably measure gain given the absence
of reliable standardised test results (i.e., IQ tests) to control for initial differences
in reading achievement.
Because of time and financial constraints at the beginning of the project, a
monitoring period of 12 months was allowed. During this period, it was planned
that all third form pupils at the experimental and control schools would be given
'inferential' reading comprehension "cloze" tests. In addition, the top class at the
experimental school (3A) was to receive a Silent Reading intervention treatment.
The reason for this study within a study, was to see if the 3A Silent Reading
experimental group gained more in reading comprehension achievement, by being
exposed on a regular basis to organised Silent Reading periods, than those who
were not. This was to provide further confirmation for the "Book Flood" effect
reported by Elley and Mangubhai (1981) in Fiji schools.
This notwithstanding, a necessary pre-requisite for the present study was the
establishment and maintenance of a school/community library together with the
introduction of a wide selection of interesting fiction and non-fiction books,
63

primarily in English7. Such a library was provided by UNESCO funding at the
beginning of the study by arrangement with the school, the Western Samoa
Department of Education and the National Library of Western Samoa.
Samples: Country and Schools
Western Samoa was chosen as the venue of the research for a number of reasons.
First, it was outside Fiji where much of the IOE's research efforts had been
directed up to that time. Second, it extended the studies on Book Flooding to a
country where the mother tongue was not English and yet where English was
crucial for academic studies in the secondary school. Third, there were good
educational support agencies such as an extensive and well run national library
service and on-the-spot coordination of the research with the library service by one
of the researchers (an ex-IOE Fellow, Dick Bishop). Fourth, the Western Samoa
Department of Education was prepared to give advice and provide human
resources for the project. Finally, Savaii (a big island separated from the main
population centres on Upolu) provided an ideal rural location where two schools,
using the same curriculum, were sufficiently isolated by geography and family
groupings to make regular contact between pupils and teachers unlikely.
On the advice of the Department of Education a school library was established in
the junior high school in the village of Fagamalo on the island of Savaii. Once
this was done, the main consideration was the question of whether the physical
presence of a library in a school made a difference to the reading comprehension
skill levels of the pupils exposed to such a significant language resource as a new
library.
Because the village of Fagamalo was about as rural a setting as one could find on
the two main islands of Western Samoa, and situated well away from the main
population centre of Apia, it was expected that pupils at Itu-o-Tane JHS No.l
would have had minimum exposure to the English language, even though the
secondary school curriculum in Western Samoa at the time required the pupils to
Both were supplied prior to starting the study. The school provided the library (the staff gave up their staff
room) and the researchers and the Western Samoa National Library provided more than 500 books. Specific details
are contained in the full-scale report mentioned earlier.
64

be able to use the English language competently enough to sit the New Zealand
School Certificate and New Zealand University Entrance examinations in the fifth
and sixth forms.
Itu-o-Tane JHS No. 1 was chosen as the experimental school and Savaii Sisifo (a
school on the other side of the island) was chosen as the control. The fact that the
schools were not the same in terms of their reading pretest scores meant that a
statistical adjustment had to be employed to equate the schools on entry levels for
reading comprehension. How this was done is outlined below.
Test Instruments
Third form pupils in the experimental and control schools were given a reading
comprehension test made up of four graded cloze extracts (See Appendix A). The
use of cloze tests for this purpose is well documented in the literature (Taylor,
1953; Bormuth, 1967; EUey, 1977). A good account of the validity, reliability and
procedure for administering cloze tests is contained in Gilmore and Wagner
(1985). The interpretation of the ensuing scores when used to determine literacy
levels is explained in Wagner (1986).
Administration
The cloze tests were administered to all third formers in both the control and the
experimental schools as parallel pre- and post-tests, initially after six months
(Post-Test I). Later, when the project was extended from 12 to 18 months8, the
cloze tests were once again administered to the same pupils at both schools after
18 months (Post-Test II). In all cases the pupils at both schools were unaware
that they were going to be tested until the researchers actually arrived at the
schools.
Unlike the other third formers at both schools, 3A at Itu-o-Tane JHS No. 1 were
required to do 15 minutes of Silent Reading every day, gradually increasing to 30
This is explained in Wagner, Bishop and Te'o (1991). In brief it is related to the need for longer periods in
which to allow the "effects" of experimental treatments of this kind to become conspicuous. As mentioned above, the
library did not open to the public until the beginning of 1985 which effectively set the study back by six months.
65

minutes daily. Selected reading books were to be taken into the classroom for
these reading sessions. New books were to be added to this selection at the
discretion of the classroom teacher and the teacher/librarian. The "Rest" of the
Itu-o-Tane JHS No. 1 third formers were to have access to the library through
teacher-initiated visits or they were able to use the library, when it was open, as
recreational readers.
On 10 May 1984, the second author visited both the experimental and control
schools in Savaii to administer the cloze tests. On 12-13 November 1984 another
visit was made to the two schools to administer the first retests (Post-Test I) in
reading, and one year later a third visit was made to both schools to administer the
second retests (Post-Test II). On the latter occasion, the silent reading intervention
was dropped from the study for the reasons mentioned elsewhere in this paper.
Data Analysis
Cloze tests were marked as either "right" or "wrong" with the "right" answer being
the exact replacement of the deleted words. A student's total score on the four
cloze sub-tests was the number of correct replacements. When classifying the
pupils' actual performances in terms of their literacy levels the following
categories were used (Wagner, 1986): Frustrational = 0-34%; Instructional = 35-
49%; Independent = 50% and above.
The method for measuring gain scores (technically 'residual gain' scores) is the
same as the method reported and used in Elley and Mangubhai (1981, p. 16). Each
student's gain score was the difference between his/her predicted score (based on
the pre-test cloze score) and his/her actual results in the two spaced post-tests.
Again, as in the case of the aforementioned research, a constant was added to each
pupil's score to eliminate negative scores during calculations.
Dropouts during the test period: In determining gain over a long period of time
(i.e. 18 months), consideration had to be given to school dropouts and absences
from testing sessions. A check was made on these kinds of changes in the
composition of the sample over time. In terms of dropouts as a percentage of
original numbers, Itu-o-Tane JHS No.l had 16.7 percent and Savaii Sisifo 13.9
percent. In other words, slightly more pupils dropped out of Itu-o-Tane JHS No.l
66

than from Savaii Sisifo. The difference between the two schools was equivalent
to almost two pupils; a figure of little overall significance.
A further check on the November 1984 cloze scores of the dropouts from both
schools compared to the scores of the ones who remained at school until the end
of 1985, showed that there was again very little difference between the "stayers"
and the "leavers" (mean=16.85, sd=9.77 versus mean=16.82, sd=7.59 for Itu-o-
Tane JHS No.l and mean=22.48, sd=9.13 versus mean=23.64, sd=9.41 for Savaii
Sisifo). To all intents and purposes the two schools were almost the same in terms
of dropout.
Absentees at Itu-o-tane JHS No.l - November 1985: Disciplinary action over the
breaking of a school rule the day before final cloze retesting led to temporary
suspensions from classes by the principal of the experimental school. This meant
that a number of test subjects were absent on the day of Post-Test II. As
indicated above, 14 pupils (19.4%) were missing from Itu-o-Tane JHS No.l when
post-testing took place in November 1985. This was a definite set-back to the
testing programme and could have had an adverse affect upon the results.
Nevertheless, as demonstrated below, the overall effect of absenteeism was
considered to have had a minimal effect upon the final results.
Results
A visit to Fagamalo during the early part of 1985 confirmed an earlier suspicion
that the Silent Reading study at Itu-o-Tane JHS No. 1 JHS may have been
corrupted. Upon investigation it transpired that the Form 3A English language
teacher responsible for the Silent Reading intervention, had not fully complied
with the wishes of the researchers, certainly during the period May 1984 to Nov
1984. Consequently, the results of the Silent Reading study were judged
unreliable and have been dropped from this study.
While there was little raw score gain in cloze score results for the experimental
school during the period May 1984 to November 1984, a raw score analysis of the
Post-Test II (May 1984 - November 1985) results indicated that the top class in
the experimental school (now 4A) showed an increase in the number of pupils
moving to the Instructional level from 4 out of 26 on the Pre-Test to 10 out of 19
67

on Post-Test II. Apart from the top class at the Control school (now also 4A)
which showed a smaller increase in number at the Instructional level (from 10 out
of 25 to 13 out of 25) over the same period, all other classes at both schools
showed no further gains.
Although the analysis of raw scores is one way to measure gain in reading
comprehension during the test period, it is a coarse and inaccurate method which
does not take into account the different levels of initial student ability which in
this study were very different. A "residual" gain measure is a relatively simple
technique for use in studies of this kind.
Using gain scores over the longer period of time - May 84 to November 85 - the
experimental school showed a significant gain in reading comprehension levels as
can be seen from Table 1.
Table 1
Cloze Mean Residual Gain Scores
for May 1984/Nov 1985 Period9
School
Class
N
Mean
SD
Itu-o-Tane
4A
19
25.29
5.57
Itu-o-Tane
4-Rest
27
19.45
4.96
Itu-o-Tane
Total
46
21.86
5.96
Savaii Sisifo
3A
25
19.79
5.76
Savaii Sisifo
3B
23
18.53
6.34
Savaii Sisifo
3C
20
17.66
4.38
Savaii Sisifo
Total
68
18.74
5.67
[School Total: t=2.8, df=112, p<.005]
In all the cloze test results, reported scores are out of a possible total of 87 items spread over four sub-
tests.
68

The Totals difference between the two schools is quite clearly in favour of Itu-o-
Tane JHS No.l and that difference is significant to a marked degree (p<.005).
Furthermore, comparing the top experimental school class (Itu-o-Tane 4A)
indicated that there was a significant difference greater than chance in favour of
the experimental school [t=2.21, df=42, p<.025].
It should be noted from Table 1 that while the top class in the experimental school
is markedly ahead of the top class in the control school, the Rest at the
experimental school also made substantial gains by comparison with the control
school. The following figure illustrates the differences between the two schools
particularly well.
Note that drawing a line across both graphs at the 20 mean cloze score level will
highlight the differences between the two schools.
Control School
Experimental School
4A Exp 4R Exp
Classes and Total
Classes and Total
Figure 1: Cloze Mean Gain Differences for Period May 1984/Nov 1985
These findings notwithstanding, there still remained the suspicion that the rather
69

large number of absentees from the November 1985 testing session at Itu-o-Tane
JHS No.l may have had a bearing upon the results. To check this possibility, the
November 1984 gain scores, of those who were absent in November 1985, were
compared with those who remained. As indicated in the full report of this study,
it was determined that the number of absentees from Itu-o-Tane JHS No.l, while
not having had a significant influence upon the school's Post-Test II mean gain
score results, could have had a small influence in the hypothesised direction.
Thus, it was not unreasonable to speculate that if there had been no absentees
from the experimental school, the experimental school's overall total, as well as
those of the individual classes at the same school, would probably have increased
the mean gain scores over 18 months to produce a slightly higher significant
difference than the one reported here.
Discussion
The results of the present study suggest that the Itu-o-Tane JHS No.l school
pupils' interaction with their school library had a marked effect upon the pupils'
reading skills during the period under study. To be more specific, when a well-
stocked and comprehensive library was made available to the pupils of Itu-o-Tane
JHS No.l, in the rural village of Fagamalo, the 1984 third form cohort showed
marked and statistically significant gains in English language comprehension over
an eighteen-month period when compared to a third form cohort in a control
school.
In terms of the hypotheses for this study, there is clear support for the first
(Introducing a school/community library would improve ESL skills), but little
support for the second (The top third form class would make even greater gains
with a formal silent reading intervention) even though there were still impressive
gains in reading comprehension by the top experimental class. When the overall
performance of the top experimental class is compared to the Rest of the same
school (who also made appreciable gains during the 18 month test period), one is
left with the impression that these gains must have been due to some form of
involvement with the school/community library. What exactly that involvement
was, apart from the physical presence and use of the library , is not clear10.
The impression is that there was very little use of the library in the first six months, of its existence.
70

In determining what form this involvement took is not a straight-forward exercise
considering the difficulties faced by the researchers in gathering valid and reliable
data for this study. For example, there were difficulties in assessing pupil reading
comprehension achievement (associated with longitudinal studies of this kind) and
with absentees and dropouts taking their toll on the original numbers in the
samples. Nevertheless, careful checks on the potential for this exodus to bias the
results showed that the significant gains were genuine but probably on the
conservative side. Further, the use of a control school and statistical procedures
involving residual gain made it possible to equate the schools on treatment
induced reading comprehension achievement by cancelling out the influence of
developmental growth in ability and to control for other common forms of
extraneous variance.
Elley and Mangubhai (1981) found that "the solution to faster growth requires
more than the presence of books in the classroom. The teacher must take an
active part in getting the pupils to read them. It is also clear that in these 12
schools, rapid progress did not occur without a greater emphasis on reading."11
Thus, as far as the present study goes, the simple presence of school library alone
could not have made the difference in the performance of the 1984 Itu-o-Tane
JHS No. 1 cohort. There were probably a number of factors which all combined to
focus the pupils' attention on the school/community library and, as a consequence,
lift the general level of the experimental school pupils' ESL skills.
As the source of a large number of interesting books, and with a lending
procedure in place (especially in the school year) to give the pupils access to the
library so that they could take books home to read, the physical presence and
relatively easy access to a school/community library must be credited with playing
a significant part in encouraging pupils in the experimental school to benefit from
their library experience in a statistically significant way.
The very fact that the school had a fine library, which the teachers and the
community were obviously proud of, all went towards placing a greater emphasis
on, and respect for, reading. Consequently, because most of the books were in
" See Elley, W.B. and Mangubhai, F. (1981) The Impact of a Book Flood in Fiji Primary
Schools. Studies in South Pacific Education, No.l, Wellington: NZCER & IOE/USP, p.29.
71

English and English is the language of the upper secondary school "leaving"
examinations, it would appear that it was the pupils' ESL reading comprehension
skills that were enhanced by the school/community library.
Conclusion
The introduction and maintenance of a school/community library in a junior
secondary school in Fagamalo, Western Samoa, appears to have had a marked
positive influence upon the ESL reading comprehension skills of its pupils when
tested over an 18-month period. One can only speculate about the possible
benefits of the Silent Reading programme had it been systematically implemented
by the teacher of 3A at Itu-o-Tane JHS No.l in the first six months of the study,
but significant progress in ESL ability was noted in spite of this setback.
The gains in reading comprehension by the experimental school, particularly the
experimental top class, in this study adds more evidence to support the assertion
that flooding a school with interesting books, no matter what the language,
markedly improves pupils' language skills (Elley, 1991). In addition, this study
showed that administering a Book Flood through a school/community library is an
effective way of improving skills in reading comprehension in the long term and,
in essence, gives further support for the practice of Book Hooding in a variety of
different contexts.
Bibliography
Bormuth, J.R. (1967) Implications and Use of the Cloze Procedure in the Evaluation
of Instructional Programmes. Occasional Report No. 3, Centre for Study of
Evaluation of Instructional Programmes, Los Angeles, University of
California.
Chomsky, C. (1979) "Language and Meaning". Applied Linguistics and Reading, ed.
R.E.Shafer, Newark:Delaware, IRA.
De'Ath, P.R.T. (1980) "The Shared Book Experience and ESL". Directions, No.4,
USP: Suva, pp. 13-22.
72

Elley, W.B. (1977) "Hanging Out the Cloze". Set 4, Wellington: NZCER.
Elley, W.B. (1980) "Studies of English Reading Levels in Fiji". In Human
Development in the South Pacific, ed. R.A.C. Stewart, Suva: Extension
Studies USP.
Elley, W.B. (1991) "Literacy Through Book Flooding: Evidence From Around the
World". Language Learning, Vol. 41, No. 2.
Elley, W.B. Barham, I.H. Lamb, H. and Wyllie, M. (1979) The Role of Grammar in
a Secondary School Curriculum. Wellington: New Zealand Council for
Educational Research.
Elley, W.B. and Mangubhai F. (1979) "A Research Report on Reading in Fiji". In
Fiji English Teachers Journal, No.15, pp. 1-7.
Elley, W.B. and Achal, S. (1980) The Development of Standardized Tests of
Achievement for Fiji. Institute of Education Report, USP, Suva.
Elley, W.B. and Mangubhai, F. (1981) "The Impact of a Book Flood in Fiji Primary
Schools". Studies in South Pacific Education, No.l. Wellington: NZCER &
IOE/USP.
Elley, W.B. and Mangubhai, F. (1981) "The Impact of a Book Flood in Fiji Primary
Schools". Studies in South Pacific Education, No.l. Wellington: NZCER
& IOE/USP.
Gilmore, A. and Wagner, G.A. (1985) The Readability of Trade Examinations.
Wellington: NZCER.
Lado, R. (1977) "Why Not Start Reading Earlier?" In Viewpoints on English as a
Second Language, ed. M.Burt, H. Dulay, and Finocchario, NY: Regents
Publishing Company.
Moore, B. (1986) "Literacy and Children's Books in the South Pacific Region".
Directions 16, Vol.8 Nos. 1 & 2, pp. 5-10, December.
73

Ragni, J. (1979) "Primary and Secondary Schools Library Questionnaire: Results and
Recommendations". In Fiji English Teachers Journal, No. 15, pp. 23-7.
Smith, F. (1978) Reading. Cambridge: CUP.
Stamp, D. (1979) "A Readability Survey of UNDP Material Using the Cloze
Procedure". Directions, No. 2, USP: Suva, pp. 23-8.
Stanovich, K.E. (1986) "Matthew Effects in Reading: Some Consequences of
Individual Differences in the Acquisition of Literacy". Reading Research
Quarterly, Vol. XXI, No.4, 360-407.
Taylor, W. (1953) "Cloze Procedure: A New Tool for Measuring Readability".
Journalism Quarterly, 30, 415-433.
Tate, G.M. (1971) Teaching Structure: A Teacher's Handbook. Wellington, New
Zealand: A.H. and A.W. Reid.
Wagner, G.A. (1986) "Interpreting Cloze Scores in the Assessment of Text
Readability and Reading Comprehension". Directions 16, Vol.8 Numbers
1&2, December.
Wagner, Graham, Bishop, Dick and Te'o, Mataina (1991) "The Fagamalo
School/Community Library Research Project", Studies in South Pacific
Education No. 5, NZCER/IOE-USP, December 1987 (Revised July 1991).
74